back <<
home
poetry
art
links
The Mozart Effect and its Limited Implications for Education.
by Natalie Baird

The argument for
The arguement against
Conclusion
Works Cited

There have been many studies over the 1990s as to the effect of Mozart’s music on student’s abilities to process tasks. However there have been only a few that have been successful in proving the so-called “Mozart Effect” (Cockerton et al., 1997; Newman et al., 1995; Rauscher et al, 1993; Steele et al., 1997). In response to the academic backlash, one of the key researchers Rauscher claimed in 1998 that to replicate the successful investigation into the Mozart effect, each variable must be kept very limited. This essay will take the view that the Mozart effect is not a phenomenon able to be taken into the normal teaching environment, as it only affects spatial-temporal reasoning, and even then under strict circumstances (Larkin, 1999; Rauscher and Shaw, 1998).

THE ARGUMENT FOR:
Research materials reporting the effect of background music on certain tasks have not exclusively tested the Mozart effect, but based research on Rauscher and Shaw’s experiments with Mozart’s music.

A study by Cockerton et. al (1997) tested the effect of music produced by a computer software package, Koan Plus. The researchers tried to replicate the study conducted by Rauscher and Shaw (1998) while replacing Mozart’s sonata for two pianos in D major (K448) with Koan produced music, which is based on Japanese Buddhist philosophy. This study successfully proved that listening to music while participants were taking a general intelligence test improved their ability to answer a greater amount of questions correctly as opposed to the condition when no music was played. Instead of arousing the participants as a way of improving intelligence test performance, the research found that Koan Plus produced music had an “attention focusing” and relaxing effect.
Cheek and Smith (1999) conducted a study claiming that music could influence achievement. It was thought that children who undertook music instruction achieved better in mathematics, perhaps due to time signature and fractional concepts within music. Those students learning the keyboard or being instructed privately were achieving the best out of the whole group interviewed.

Iwaki et al. claimed that certain types of music were more stimulating and therefore increased brain activity (1997). However this study was not conducted using Mozart, but Holst’s “The Planets”, and measured frontal lobe activity in the brain not performance in intelligence tests. Using two selections “Mars” and “Venus” from “The Planets”, Iwaki et al. claimed that certain pieces of music aroused and calmed the frontal lobe according to melody, rhythm and tempo. The researchers suggested that this type of activity in the brain has positive implications for music therapy, however not for educational purposes (1997).
Godeli et al. researched the effect on music in a pre-school environment – not on performance within intelligence tests (1996). Music, including German, French and American folk songs and heavy metal music by Def Leopard, was found to stimulate social interactions between young children. The research suggests that background music could promote different relationship structures within the classroom, that of child to child rather than teacher to child.

top

THE ARGUMENT AGAINST:
The most reliable research investigating the effects of background music and task performance did test the Mozart effect, and did not test variations.
It has been found that Mozart’s sonata (K448) actually did improve ability to process spatial-temporal tasks, however only on a temporary basis (Iwaki et al., 1997; Larkin, 1999). An article by Larkin suggests that any effect on performance is most probably mood or arousal rather than an indicator of higher brain function and any effect will dissipate in a matter of minutes after listening to Mozart (1999). This is supported by Iwaki et al. who claim that while there was an increase in brain activity while listening to music, the orienting response to the music declines rapidly after the first few minutes of listening to the piece (1995).
Steele et al. have found that Mozart did not have any effect on backwards digit span performance (1997). The researchers concede that the dependant measure of performance in response to the music may have been related to their failure. However, they claim that other experiments used precisely the same dependant measure, that is, paper folding and cutting tasks, as the original study (Rauscher et al., 1993), yet failed to achieve the same result.
One study that replicated the procedure of Rauscher et al. (1993), came up with no evidence as to the benefits of listening to Mozart to improve spatial reasoning (Newman et al., 1995). Even making the sample size considerably larger to account for thorough testing, prior musical knowledge and musical preference failed to make this experiment succeed.

top

CONCLUSION
To achieve successful results when testing the Mozart effect, even the primary researchers Rauscher and Shaw (1998) admit that certain factors must be the same as in their original study. This means that dependant measures, the order of conditions and selection of musical composition must be in line so that the study will not fail. Other researchers have tried to change the variables in order to test the boundaries of this effect (Newman et al., 1995; Steele et al., 1997) with few successful results. However the research that introduced certain variables such as different music and different tasks have found that Mozart and indeed other musical compositions can have an effect on mood, attention and relaxation (Iwaki et al., 1997; Larkin, 1999; Newton et al., 1995). This effect has been proven to be temporary only however these aspects can be applied to the classroom much easier and with more realistic results.

top

 


WORKS CITED

Cheek, J.M and Smith, L.R. (1999). “Music Training and Mathematics Achievement”. Adolescence, 34, Winter, 759 – 761.

Cockerton, T., Moore, S. and Norman, D. (1997). “Cognitive Test Performance and Background Music”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, November

Godeli M.R, Santana, P.R, Souza, V.H, Marquetti, G.P. (1996). “Influence of Background Music on Preschoolers’ Behaviour: A Naturalistic Approach”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, April, 1123 – 1129.

Iwaki, T, Hayashi, M, Hori, T. (1997). “Changes in Alpha Band EEG Activity in the Frontal Area After Stimulation with Music of Different Affective Content”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, January, 515 – 526.

Larkin, M. (1999, August 28). “Mozart effect” comes under strong fire. The Lancet.

Newman, J, Rosenbach, J, Burns, K, Latimer, B.C, Matocha, H.R, Rosenthal Vogt, E. (1995). “An Experimental Test of ‘The Mozart Effect’: Does Listening to His Music Improve Spatial Ability?”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81, November, 1379 – 1387.

Rauscher, F.H, Shaw, G.L and Ky, K.N. (1993). “Music and spatial task performance”. Nature, 365, 611.

Rauscher, F.H and Shaw, G.L. (1998). “Key Components of the Mozart Effect”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, March, 835 – 841.

Rideout, B.E, Dougherty, S, Wernert, L. (1998). “Effect of Music on Spatial Performance: A Test of Generality”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, February, 512 – 514.

Serebriakoff, V. (1988). A Guide to Intelligence and Personality Testing: Including Actual Tests and Answers. London: Camforth.

Steele, K.M, Ball, T.N and Runk, R. (1997). “Listening to Mozart does not Enhance Backwards Digit Span Performance”. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, March, 1179 – 1184.

top